Publications

 

 

2016

Morton , F. B., Brosnan, S. F., Prétôt , L., Buchanan-Smith , H. M., O'sullivan , E., Stocker, M., & Wilson , V. A. D. (2016). Using photographs to study animal social cognition and behaviour: Do capuchins' responses to photos reflect reality? Behavioural Processes, 38–46.

Asakawa-Haas, K., Schiestl, M., Bugnyar, T., & Massen, J. J. M. (2016). Partner Choice in Raven (Corvus corax) Cooperation. PLoS ONE, 11(6), [e0156962].

Bodony, D. J., Day, L. D., R. Friscia, A., Fusani, L., Karon, A., W. Swenson, G., ... Schlinger, B. A. (2016). Determination of the wingsnap sonation mechanism of the golden-collared manakin (Manacus vitellinus). Journal of Experimental Biology,  219, 1524-1534. DOI: doi:10.1242/jeb.128231

Bugnyar, T., Reber, S., & Buckner, C. (2016). Ravens attribute visual access to unseen competitors. Nature Communications, 7, [10506 (2016)].

Deventer, S., Uhl, F., Bugnyar, T., Miller, R., Fitch, W. T. S., Schiestl, M., ... Christine, S. (2016). Behavioural Type Affects Space Use in a Wild Population of Crows (Corvus corone). Ethology, 122(11), 881–891.

Filippi, P., Ocklenburg, S., Bowling, D., Heege, L., Güntürkün, O., Newen, A. . R-U-B., & de Boer, B. (2016). More than words (and faces): evidence for a Stroop effect of prosody in emotion word processing. Cognition & Emotion, 1-13.

Fischmeister, F., Martins, M., Beisteiner, R., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2016). Self-similarity and recursion as default modes in human cognition. Cortex: journal devoted to study of the nervous system and behavior, 1-19.

Fitch, W. T. S., Suthers, R. A., Fay, R. R., & Popper, A. N. (2016). Vertebrate Sound Production and Acoustic Communication. Springer.

Fitch, W. T. S. (2016). Dance, Music, Meter and Groove: A Forgotten Partnership. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10(64).

Fitch, W. T. S. (2016). Why formal semantics and primate communication make strange bedfellows. Theoretical Linguistics, 42(1-2), 97-109.

Fitch, W. T. S. (2016). Linguistics: Sound and meaning in the world's languages. Nature, 539, [39–40].

Fitch, W. T. S. (2016). Vertebrate Bioacoustics: Prospects and Open Problems. in Vertebrate Sound Production and Acoustic Communication. (Band 53, S. 297-328). ( Springer Handbook of Auditory Research ; Band 53). Springer.

Fitch, W. T. S., de Boer, B., Mathur, N., & Ghazanfar, A. A. (2016). Monkey vocal tracts are speechready. Science Advances, 2, [ e1600723].

Fitch, W. T. S. (2016). The Myth of Mirror Neurons. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 91(3), 368-369.

Fitch, W. T. S. (2016). Reinventing Linguistics – Again. Inference: International Review of Science, 2(3), 1-10.

Fitch, W. T. S., Mathur, N., de Boer, B., & Ghazanfar, A. A. (2016). Monkey vocal tracts are speech-ready. Science Advances.

Fitch, W. T. S., & Suthers, R. A. (2016). Vertebrate vocal production: An introductory overview. in Vertebrate Sound Production and Acoustic Communication . (S. 1-18). Springer.

Fitch, W. T. S. (2016). Laryngeal Descent. in Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science . (S. 1-4). Springer.

Frischauf, C., Gockert, R., Kavcik-Graumann, N., & Rabeder, G. (2016). "Kiskevély knifes" indicate the menu of Alpine cave bears - Comparative studies on wedge shaped defects of canines and incisors. Cranium, 33, 14-17.

Gallup, A. C., & Massen, J. J. M. (2016). There is no difference in contagious yawning between men and women. The Royal Society Open Science.

Garcia, M., Gingras, B., Bowling, D., Herbst, C., Böckle, M., Locatelli, Y., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2016). Structural classification of Wild Boar (Sus scrofa) vocalizations. Ethology.

Garcia, M., Wondrak, M., Huber, L., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2016). Honest signaling in domestic piglets (Sus scrofa domesticus): vocal allometry and the information content of grunt calls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General.

Geambasu, A., Ravignani, A., & Levelt, C. C. (2016). Preliminary Experiments on Human Sensitivity to Rhythmic Structure in a Grammar with Recursive Self-Similarity. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 10(281).

Güntürkün, O., & Bugnyar, T. (2016). Cognition without Cortex. Trends in Cognitive Science, 20(4), 291–303.

Hahn, A., Höschele, M., M. Guillette, L., Hoang, J., McMillan, N., Congdon, J., ... Sturdy, C. (2016). Black-capped chickadees categorize songs based on features that vary geographically. Animal Behaviour, 93-104. DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.11.017

Hampala, V., Garcia, M., Svec, J., Scherer, R. C., & Herbst, C. (2016). Relationship Between the Electroglottographic Signal and Vocal Fold Contact Area. Journal of Voice, 161-171. DOI: 10.1016/j.jvoice.2015.03.018

Horn, L., Scheer, C., Bugnyar, T., & Massen, J. J. M. (2016). Proactive prosociality in a cooperatively breeding corvid, the azure-winged magpie (Cyanopica cyana). Biology Letters, [20160649]. DOI: 10.1098/rsbl.2016.0649

Huber, L., & O'Hara, M. (2016). Extractive Foraging Hypothesis, The (Parker and Gibson 1997, 2015). in Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science. Springer International Publishing AG .

Höschele, M., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2016). Phonological perception by birds: budgerigars can perceive lexical stress. Animal Cognition, 19(3), 643–654.

Höschele, M., & Bowling, D. (2016). “Sex differences in rhythmic preferences in the budgerigar (Melopsittacus undulatus): A comparative study with humans”. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, [1543].

Kulahci, I. G., Rubenstein, D. I., Bugnyar, T., Hoppitt, W., Mikus, N., & Schwab, C. (2016). Social networks predict selective observation and information spread in ravens. The Royal Society Open Science, [ 160256].

Laumer, I., Bugnyar, T., & Auersperg, A. (2016). Flexible decision-making relative to reward quality and tool functionality in Goffin cockatoos (Cacatua goffiniana). Scientific Reports, 6, [28380].

Loretto, M-C., Schuster, R., & Bugnyar, T. (2016). GPS tracking of non-breeding ravens reveals the importance of anthropogenic food sources during their dispersal in the Eastern Alps. Current Zoology, 1-8.

Lupi, S., Goymann, W., Cardinale, M., & Fusani, L. (2016). Physiological conditions influence stopover behaviour of short-distance migratory passerines. Journal of Ornithology, 157(2), 583–589.

Massen, J. J. M., Slipogor, V., & Gallup, A. C. (2016). An Observational Investigation of Behavioral Contagion in Common Marmosets (Callithrix jacchus): Indications for Contagious Scent-Marking. Frontiers in Psychology, [1190].

Miller, R., Laskowski, K., Schiestl, M., Bugnyar, T., & Schwab, C. (2016). Socially Driven Consistent Behavioural Differences during Development in Common Ravens and Carrion Crows. PLoS ONE, 11(2), [10.1371/journal.pone.0148822].

Miller, R., Schwab, C., & Bugnyar, T. (2016). Explorative Innovators and Flexible Use of Social Information in Common Ravens (Corvus corax) and Carrion Crows (Corvus corone). Journal of Comparative Psychology.

Munteanu, M. A., Starnberger, I., Pasukonis, A., Bugnyar, T., Hödl, W., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2016). Take the long way home: Behaviour of a neotropical frog, Allobates femoralis, in a detour task. Behavioural Processes.

Narins, P., Stöger-Horwath, A., & O’Connell-Rodwell, C. (2016). Infrasonic and Seismic Communication in the Vertebrates with Special Emphasis on the Afrotheria: An Update and Future Directions. in Vertebrate Sound Production and Acoustic Communication. (S. 191-227). ( Springer Handbook of Auditory Research). Springer.

Oh, J., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2016). CATOS (Computer Aided Training/Observing System): Automating animal observation and training. Behavior Research Methods, 1-11. DOI: 10.3758/s13428-015-0694-9

Olkowicz, S., Kocourek, M., Lucan, R. K., Porteš, M., Fitch, W. T. S., Herculano-Houze, S., & Nemec, P. (2016). Birds have primate-like numbers of neurons in the forebrain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America (PNAS), 113(26), 7255–7260.

Preininger, D., Handschuh, S., Boeckle, M., Sztatecsny, M., & Hoedl, W. (2016). Comparison of female and male vocalisation and larynx morphology in the size dimorphic foot-flagging frog species Staurois guttatus. Herpetological Journal, 26(3), 187-197.

Ravignani, A., Fitch, W. T. S., Hanke , F. D., Heinrich, T., Hurgitsch, B., Kotz, S., ... Stöger-Horwath, A. (2016). What Pinnipeds Have to Say about Human Speech, Music, and the Evolution of Rhythm. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 10.

Reber, S., Böckle, M., Szipl, G., Janisch, J., Bugnyar, T., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2016). Territorial raven pairs are sensitive to structural changes in simulated acoustic displays of conspecifics. Animal Behaviour, 116, 153–162.

Ringler, E., Pasukonis, A., Ringler, M., & Huber, L. (2016). Sex-specific offspring discrimination reflects respective risks and costs of misdirected care in a poison frog. Animal Behaviour, 114, 173-179. DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.02.008

Schmitt, V., Federspiel, I., Eckert, J., Keupp, S., Tschernek, L., Faraut, L., ... Fischer, J. (2016). Do monkeys compare themselves to others? Animal Cognition.

Schwing, R., Jocteur, E., Wein, A., Noe, R., & Massen, J. J. M. (2016). Kea cooperate better with sharing affiliates. Animal Cognition, 1-10.

Sima, M., Pika, S., & Bugnyar, T. (2016). Experimental Manipulation of Food Accessibility Affects Conflict Management Behaviour in Ravens. Ethology, 122(2), 114–126.

Slana, A., Repovs, G., Fitch, W. T. S., & Gingras, B. (2016). Harmonic context influences pitch class equivalence judgments through gestalt and congruency effects. Acta Psychologica, 166, 54–63.

Slipogor, V., Gunhold-de Oliveira, T., Tadic, Z., & Massen, J. J. M. (2016). Consistent inter-individual differences in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) in Boldness-Shyness, Stress-Activity, and Exploration-Avoidance. American Journal of Primatology, 78(9), 961–973.

Stocker, M., Munteanu, M. A., Stöwe, M., Schwab, C., Palme, R., & Bugnyar, T. (2016). Loner or socializer? Ravens' adrenocortical response to individual separation depends on social integration. Hormones and Behavior. DOI: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.11.009

Stöger-Horwath, A., & Baotic, A. (2016). Information content and acoustic structure of male African elephant social rumbles. Scientific Reports, [ 27585 ].

Szabo, B., Bugnyar, T., & Auersperg, A. (2016). Within-group relationships and lack of social enhancement during object manipulation in captive Goffin’s cockatoos (Cacatua goffiniana). Learning and Behavior. DOI: 10.3758/s13420-016-0235-0

Tebbich, S., Griffin, A. S., Peschl, F-M., & Sterelny, K. (2016). From mechanisms to function: an integrated framework of animal innovation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Biological Sciences, 1-11. DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0195

Zaccaroni, M., Della Seta, D., Farabollini, F., Fusani, L., & Dessi-Fulgheri, F. (2016). Developmental Exposure to Very Low Levels of Ethynilestradiol Affects Anxiety in a Novelty Place Preference Test of Juvenile Rats. Neurotoxicity Research, 1–10.

2015


Kalcher-Sommersguter, E., Preuschoft, S., Franz-Schaider, C., Hemelrijk, C., Crailsheim, K., & Massen, J. J. M. (2015). Early maternal loss affects social integration of chimpanzees throughout their lifetime. Scientific Reports, 5. DOI: 10.1038/srep16439

Auersperg, A., van Horik, J. O., Bugnyar, T., Kacelnik, A., Emery, N., & von Bayern, A. (2015). Combinatory Actions During Object Play in Parrots (Psittacus erithacus) and Corvids (Corvus). Journal of Comparative Psychology. DOI:  dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038314

Auersperg, A. (2015). Exploration technique and technical innovations in corvids and parrots. in Animal Innovation and Creativity. (S. 45-69). Elsevier Academic Press.

Aust, U., & Braunöder, E. (2015). Transfer between local and global processing levels by pigeons (Columba livia) and humans (Homo sapiens) in exemplar- and rule-based categorization tasks. Journal of Comparative Psychology. DOI: 10.1037/a0037691

Baotic, A., Stöger-Horwath, A., & Sicks, F. (2015). Nocturnal “humming” vocalizations: adding a piece to the puzzle of giraffe vocal communication. BMC Research Notes, 8, [425]. DOI: 10.1186/s13104-015-1394-3

Barske, J., Schlinger, B. A., & Fusani, L. (2015). The presence of a female influences courtship performance of male manakins. The Auk, 132, 594-603.

Bowling, D., Gingras, B., Han, S., Sundararajan, J., & Opitz, E. (2015). Tone of voice in emotional expression and its implications for the affective character of musical mode. Journal of Interdisciplinary Music Studies.

Bowling, D., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2015). Do Animal Communication Systems Have Phonemes? Trends in Cognitive Science, 19(10), 555–557.

Bowling, D., & Purves, D. (2015). A biological rationale for musical consonance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America (PNAS), 11155-11160.

Bowling, D. (2015). The problem with emotion: Comment on "The quartet theory of human emotions: An integrative and neurofunctional model" by S. Koelsch et al. Physics of Life Reviews.

Bowling, D., Garcia, M., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2015). Acoustic allometry in mammals: Body size, larynx size and call frequency. Postersitzung präsentiert bei International Bioacoustics Congress (IBAC), Murnau, Deutschland.

Dunn, J. C., Halenar, L. B., Davies, T. G., Cristobal-Azkarate , J., Reby, D., Sykes, D., ... Knapp , L. A. (2015). Evolutionary Trade-Off between Vocal Tract and Testes Dimensions in Howler Monkeys. Current Biology.

Fitch, W. T. S. (2015). Four principles of bio-musicology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Biological Sciences.

Fitch, W. T. S. (2015). How pets got their spots (and floppy ears). New Scientist: the global science and technology weekly, 225(3002), 24–25.

Fusani, L., & Gahr, M. (2015). Differential Expression of Melatonin Receptor Subtypes Mel1a, Mel1b and Mel1c in relation to Melatonin Binding in the Male Songbird Brain. Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

Gingras, B., Honing, H., Peretz, I., Trainor, L., & Fisher, S. (2015). Defining the biological bases of individual differences in musicality. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Biological Sciences. DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2014.0092

Gingras, B., Marin, M., Puig-Waldmüller, E. S., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2015). The Eye is Listening: Music-Induced Arousal and Individual Differences Predict Pupillary Responses. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00619

Gunhold-de Oliveira, T., Range, F., Huber, L., & Bugnyar, T. (2015). Long-term fidelity of foraging techniques in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). American Journal of Primatology. DOI: 10.1002/ajp.22342

Herbst, C., Oh, J., Vydrová, J., & Svec, J. (2015). DigitalVHI—a freeware open-source software application to capture the Voice Handicap Index and other questionnaire data in various languages. Logopedics Phoniatrics Vocology, 40(2).

Höschele, M., Merchant, H., Kikuchi, Y., Hattori, Y., & ten Cate, C. (2015). Searching for the origins of musicality across species. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Biological Sciences, 370.

J. Kubitza, R., Bugnyar, T., & Schwab, C. (2015). Pair bond characteristics and maintenance in free-flying jackdaws Corvus monedula: effects of social context and season. Journal of Avian Biology, 45, 206–215. [10.1111/jav.00508].

Loretto, M-C., Schuster, R., & Bugnyar, T. (2015). GPS tracking of non-breeding ravens reveals importance of anthropogenic food sources during their dispersal in the Eastern Alps. Current Zoology. DOI: 10.1093/cz/zow016

Loretto, M-C., Schuster, R., Reimann, S., Graulich, D. M., & Bugnyar, T. (2015). Shared space - individually used: Spatial behaviour of non-breeding ravens (Corvus corax) close to a permanent anthropogenic food source. Journal of Ornithology.

Lovari, S., Pokheral, C. P., Jnawali, S. R., Fusani, L., & Ferretti, F. (2015). Coexistence of the tiger and the common leopard in a prey-rich area: the role of prey partitioning. Journal of Zoology.

M. Guillette, L., Hahn, A., Höschele, M., Przyslupski, A-M., & Sturdy, C. (2015). Individual differences in learning speed, performance accuracy and exploratory behaviour in black-capped chickadees. Animal Cognition, 18(1), 165–178. DOI: 10.1007/s10071-014-0787-3

Martins, M., Muršič, Z., Oh, J., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2015). Representing visual recursion does not require verbal or motor resources. Cognitive Psychology, 77, 20–41. DOI: doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2015.01.004

Martins, M., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2015). Do we represent intentional action as recursively embedded? The answer must be empirical. A comment on Vicari and Adenzato (2014). Consciousness and Cognition.

Martins, M., Martins, I. P., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2015). A novel approach to investigate recursion and iteration in visual hierarchical processing. Behavior Research Methods.

Massen, J. J. M., Lambert, M., Schiestl, M., & Bugnyar, T. (2015). Subadult ravens generally don't transfer valuable tokens to conspecifics when there is nothing to gain for themselves. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 885. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00885

Massen, J. J. M., Church, A., & Gallup, A. C. (2015). Auditory Contagious Yawning in Humans: An Investigation into Affiliation and Status Effects. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1735. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01735

Massen, J. J. M., Ritter, C., & Bugnyar, T. (2015). Tolerance and reward equity predict cooperation in ravens (Corvus corax). Scientific Reports, 5. DOI: 10.1038/srep15021

Massen, J. J. M., Church, A., & Gallup, A. C. (2015). The influence of political affiliation and status on auditory contagious yawning in humans. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1735.

Merchant, H., Grahn, J., Trainor, L., Rohrmeier, M., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2015). Finding the beat: a neural perspective across humans and non-human primates. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Biological Sciences, 370(1664).

Miller, R., Bugnyar, T., Pölzl, K., & Schwab, C. (2015). Differences in exploration behaviour in common ravens and carrion crows during development and across social context. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 69, 1209–1220. DOI: DOI 10.1007/s00265-015-1935-8

Monti, F., Duriez, O., Arnal, V., Dominici, J-M., Sforzi, A., Fusani, L., ... Montgelard, C. (2015). Being cosmopolitan: evolutionary history and phylogeography of a specialized raptor, the Osprey Pandion haliaetus. BMC Evolutionary Biology. DOI: 10.1186/s12862-015-0535-6

O'Hara, M., Gajdon, G., & Huber, L. (2015). The advantage of objects over images in discrimination and reversal learning by kea, Nestor notabilis. Animal Behaviour, 101, 51-60.

O'Hara, M., Auersperg, A., Bugnyar, T., & Huber, L. (2015). Inference by Exclusion in Goffin Cockatoos (Cacatua goffini). PLoS ONE, 10(8). DOI:  10.1371/journal.pone.0134894

Pasukonis, A., Warrington, I., Ringler, M., & Hödl, W. (2015). Poison frogs rely on experience to find the way home in the rainforest. Biology Letters, 10(20140642). DOI: 10.1098/rsbl.2014.0642

Pasukonis, A., Loretto, M-C., Warrington, I., Ringler, M., & Hödl, W. (2015). Navigation in the rain forest: experience based homing in poison frogs. Postersitzung präsentiert bei 34th International Ethological Conference 2015, Cairns, Australien.

Ravignani, A., Westphal Fitch, G., Aust, U., Schlumpp, M., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2015). More than one way to see it: Individual heuristics in avian visual computation. Cognition, 143, 13–24. DOI: doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.021

Reber, S., Nishimura, T., Janisch, J., Robertson, M., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2015). A Chinese alligator in heliox: formant frequencies in a crocodilian. Journal of Experimental Biology, 218, 2442-2447. DOI: doi:10.1242/jeb.119552

Ringler, E., Pasukonis, A., Fitch, W. T. S., Huber, L., Hödl, W., & Ringler, M. (2015). Flexible compensation of uniparental care: female poison frogs take over when males disappear. Behavioral Ecology, 1–7. DOI:  doi:10.1093/beheco/arv069

Ringler, E., Pasukonis, A., Fitch, W. T. S., Huber, L., Hödl, W., & Ringler, M. (2015). Mommy will fix it: Behavioural flexibility and sex-biased offspring recognition in a poison frog with male parental care. Beitrag in Behaviour, Cairns, Australien.

Rößler, T. (2015). A Crow´s Approach: Inference by Exclusion and Alternative Responses. Postersitzung präsentiert bei Graduate Meeting of the “Ethologische Gesellschaft“ & “Deutsche Zoologische Gesellschaft" (Studying Animal Behaviour in the Field), Grünau im Almtal, Österreich.

Schöberl, G. I., Kortekaas, K., Schöberl, F., & Kotrschal, K. (2015). Algorithm-supported visual error correction (AVEC) of heart rate measurements in dogs, Canis lupus familiaris. Behavior Research Methods, 47(4), 1356-1364. DOI: 10.3758/s13428-014-0546-z

Skrip, M. M., Bauchinger, U., Fusani, L., McWilliams, S. R., & Goymann, W. (2015). Access to water affects the condition dependency of nocturnal restlessness in Garden Warblers on a Mediterranean island stopover. Journal of Ornithology, 1-8.

Skrip, M. M., Bauchinger, U., Goymann, W., Fusani, L., Cardinale, M., Alan, R. R., & McWilliams, S. R. (2015). Migrating songbirds on stopover prepare for, and recover from, oxidative challenges posed by long-distance flight. Ecology and Evolution, 3198–3209. DOI: 10.1002/ece3.1601

Slipogor, V., Massen, J. J. M., Millesi, E., & Bugnyar, T. (2015). Long-term vs. short-term consistency of social personality traits and salivary cortisol levels in common marmosets. Folia Primatologica, 86.

Soldatini, C., Albores-Barajas, Y. V., Tagliavia, M., Massa, B., Fusani, L., & Canoine, V. (2015). Effects of human disturbance on cave-nesting seabirds: the case of the storm petrel. Conservation Physiology. DOI: 10.1093/conphys/cov041

Sonnweber, R., Ravignani, A., & Fitch, W. T. (2015). Non-adjacent visual dependency learning in chimpanzees. Animal Cognition, 18(3), 733-745. DOI: 10.1007/s10071-015-0840-x

Sonnweber, R-S., Ravignani, A., Stobbe, N., Schiestl, G., Wallner, B., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2015). Rank-dependent grooming patterns and cortisol alleviation in Barbary macaques. American Journal of Primatology, 77(6), 688-700. DOI: 10.1002/ajp.22391

Sonnweber, R-S., Massen, J. J. M., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2015). Post-copulatory grooming: a conditional mating strategy? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 1749-1759. DOI: 10.1007/s00265-015-1987-9

Sonnweber, R-S., Ravignani, A., Stobbe, N., Schiestl, G., Wallner, B., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2015). The role of social styles in grooming patterns and stress alleviation. in Folia Primatologica. (S. 363-364)

Sterck, E. H. M., Olesen, C. U., & Massen, J. J. M. (2015). No costly prosociality among related long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 129(3), 275-282. DOI: dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0039180

Stocker, M., Munteanu, A., Stöwe, M., Schwab, C., & Bugnyar, T. (2015). Ravens’ stress levels are related to their social integration..

Stocker, M., O'sullivan , E., Sonnweber, R-S., & Caldwell, C. (2015). Capuchins’ behavioural and endocrine response to conspecifics’ actions. in Folia Primatologica.

Szipl, G., Böckle, M., Wascher, C., Spreafico, M., & Bugnyar, T. (2015). With whom to dine? Ravens' responses to food-associated calls depend on individual characteristics of the caller. Animal Behaviour, 99(1), 33-42.

Urquiza, E. G., & Kotrschal, K. (2015). The mind behind anthropomorphic thinking: attribution of mental states to other species. Animal Behaviour, 109, 167-176. DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.08.011

Westphal Fitch, G., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2015). Towards a comparative approach to empirical aesthetics. in Art, Aesthetics, and the Brain. Oxford University Press.

Zeppelbauer, M., & Stöger-Horwath, A. (2015). Establishing the fundamentals for an elephant early warning and monitoring system. BMC Research Notes, 8, [409]. DOI: 10.1186/s13104-015-1370-y

2014

Auersperg, A., von Bayern, A., Weber, S., Szabadvari, A., Bugnyar, T., & Kacelnik, A. (2014). Social transmission of tool use and tool manufacture in Goffin cockatoos (Cacatua goffini). Proceedings of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological sciences, 281(1793).

Auersperg, A., Oswald, N., Domanegg, M., Gajdon, G., & Bugnyar, T. (2014). Unrewarded object combinations in parrots. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 1(4), 470-488. 10.12966/ abc . 11.05 .201 4

Barske, J., Fusani, L., Wikelski, M., Feng, N. Y., Santos, M., & Schlinger, B. A. (2014). Energetics of the acrobatic courtship in male golden-collared manakins (Manacus vitellinus). Proceedings of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological sciences, 281(1776), 20132482. 10.1098/rspb.2013.2482

Bowling, D. (2014). Cognitive theory and brain fact: Insights for the future of cognitive neuroscience Comment on “Toward a computational framework for cognitive biology: Unifying approaches from cognitive neuroscience and comparative cognition” by W. Tecumseh Fitch. Physics of Life Reviews, 11, 377-379.

Cianchetti Benedetti, M., Fusani, L., Bonanni, R., Cardinale, M., & Carere, C. (2014). Condition-Dependent Nocturnal Hypothermia in Garden Warblers Sylvia borin at a Spring Stopover Site. Ardea, 101(2), 113-119.

Doolittle, E., Gingras, B., Endres, D., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2014). Overtone-based pitch selection in hermit thrush song: Unexpected convergence with scale construction in human music. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America (PNAS), 111(46), 16616–16621. 10.1073/pnas.1406023111

Filippi, P., Gingras, B., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2014). Pitch enhancement facilitates word learning across visual contexts. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(1468).

Filippi, P. (2014). Computo ergo sum. Review of: Sergio Balari and Guillermo Lorenzo, 2012. Comptational phenotypes: Towards an Evolutionary Developmental Biolinguistics. Biological Theory: integrating development, evolution & cognition, 9, 115-118.

Fitch, W. T. S. (2014). Information considered harmful in animal communication. Current Biology, 24(1).

Fitch, W. T. S. (2014). Toward ac omputational framework for cognitive biology: Unifying approaches from cognitive neuroscience and comparative cognition. Physics of Life Reviews, 329-364.

Fusani, L., Barske, J., Day, L. D., Fuxjager, M. J., & Schlinger, B. A. (2014). Physiological control of elaborate male courtship: female choice for neuromuscular systems. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews .

Fusani, L., Donaldson, Z., London, S. E., Fuxjager, M. J., & Schlinger, B. A. (2014). Expression of androgen receptor in the brain of a sub-oscine bird with an elaborate courtship display. Neuroscience Letters, 578, 61-65.

Fusani, L., Bertolucci, C., Frigato, E., & Foà, A. (2014). Cryptochrome expression in the eye of migratory birds depends on their migratory status. Journal of Experimental Biology, 217(6), 918-923.

Garcia, M., Wyman, M. T., Charlton, B. D., Fitch, W. T. S., & Reby, D. (2014). Response of red deer stags (Cervus elaphus) to playback of harsh versus common roars. Naturwissenschaften, 101(10), 851-854. [10.1007/s00114-014-1217-8].

Gingras, B. (2014). Individuality in music performance: Introduction to the research topic. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(661). 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00661

Gingras, B., Pohler, G., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2014). Exploring shamanic journeying: Repetitive drumming with shamanic instructions induces specific subjective experiences but no larger cortisol decrease than instrumental meditation music. PLoS ONE, 9(7). 10.1371/journal.pone.0102103

Gingras, B. (2014). Cross-species comparisons. in W. F. Thompson (Hrsg.), Music in the Social and Behavioral Sciences: An encyclopedia. (Band 1, S. 287-290). Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications Ltd..

Gingras, B. (2014). Systematic musicology. in W. F. Thompson (Hrsg.), Music in the Social and Behavioral Sciences: An encyclopedia. (Band 2, S. 1093-1095). Sage Publications Ltd..

Gunhold-de Oliveira, T., Massen, J. J. M., Schiel, N., & Souto, A. (2014). Memory, transmission and persistence of alternative foraging techniques in wild common marmosets. Animal Behaviour, 91, 79-91.

Gunhold-de Oliveira, T., Whiten, A., & Bugnyar, T. (2014). Video demonstrations seed alternative problem-solving techniques in wild common marmosets. Biology Letters, 20140439. 10.1098/rsbl.2014.0439

Herbst, C., Lohscheller, J., Svec, J., Henrich, N., Weissengruber, G., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2014). Glottal opening and closing events investigated by electroglottography and super-high-speed video recordings. Journal of Experimental Biology, 955-963.

Hillemann, F., Bugnyar, T., Kotrschal, K., & Wascher, C. (2014). Waiting for better, not for more: corvids respond to quality in two delay maintenance tasks. Animal Behaviour, 90, 1-10. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.01.007

Kubitza, R., Bugnyar, T., & Schwab, C. (2014). Pair bond characteristics and maintenance in free-flying jackdaws, Corvus monedula: effects of social context and season. Journal of Avian Biology, 45, 1-10. [10.1111/jav.00508]. 10.1111/jav.00508

Lechner, W. (2014). Young Squeaker Catfish Can Already Talk and Listen to Their Conspecifics. in G. Witzany (Hrsg.), Biocommunication of Animals. (S. 321-335). Springer Netherlands. 10.1007/978-94-007-7414-8_18

Martins, M., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2014). Hierarchical processing in music, language, and action: Lashley revisited. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 87–104.

Massen, J. J. M., & Koski, S. E. (2014). Chimps of a feather sit together: chimpanzee friendships are based on homophily in personality. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35, 1-8.

Massen, J. J. M., Szipl, G., Spreafico, M., & Bugnyar, T. (2014). Ravens intervene in others' bonding attempts. Current Biology, 24(22), 2733-2736.

Massen, J. J. M., Pasukonis, A., Schmidt, J., & Bugnyar, T. (2014). Ravens notice dominance reversals among conspecifics within and outside their social group. Nature Communications, 5, [3679]. 10.1038/ncomms4679

Massen, J. J. M., Dusch, K., Eldakar, O., & Gallup, A. (2014). A thermal window for yawning in humans: Yawning as a Brain Cooling Mechanism. Physiology & Behavior, 145-148.

Massen, J. J. M., Dusch, K., Eldakar, O., & Gallup, A. (2014). Territorial raven pairs are sensitive to structural changes in simulated duets of conspecifics. Physiology & Behavior, 130, 145-148.

Miller, R., Schiestl, M., Whiten, A., Schwab, C., & Bugnyar, T. (2014). Tolerance and Social Facilitation in the Foraging Behaviour of Free-Ranging Crows (Corvus corone corone; C. c. cornix). Ethology, 120, 1248-1255. [10.1111/eth.12298]. 10.1111/eth.12298

Mueller-Paul, J. S., Wilkinson, A., Aust, U., Steurer, M., Hall, G., & Huber, L. (2014). Touchscreen performance and knowledge transfer in the red-footed tortoise (Chelonoides carbonaria). Behavioural Processes, 187–192. 10.1016/j.beproc.2014.06.003

Müllensiefen, D., Gingras, B., Musil, J., & Stewart, L. (2014). The musicality of non-musicians: An index for assessing musical sophistication in the general population. PLoS ONE, 9(2). 10.1371/journal.pone.0089642

Pfuhl, G., Gattermayr, M., & Bugnyar, T. (2014). Will food-handling time influence agonistic behaviour in sub-adult common ravens (Corvus corax)?. Behavioural Processes, 103, 67-74. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2013.11.003

Ravignani, A., Bowling, D., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2014). Chorusing, synchrony, and the evolutionary functions of rhythm. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1118.

Ravignani, A. (2014). Chronometry for the chorusing herd: Hamilton’s legacy on context-dependent acoustic signalling—a comment on Herbers (2013). Biology Letters, 10(20131018). dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2013.1018

Rubolini, D., Fusani, L., Bonisoli-Alquati, A., Canoine, V., Caprioli, M., Romano, M., ... Saino, N. (2014). Effects of Egg and Circulating Testosterone on Ring-Necked Pheasant (Phasianus colchicus) Male Traits and Combat Outcome. Ethology, 120(1), 64-76.

Réka, K., & Gingras, B. (2014). Perceiving individuality in harpsichord performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(141), 1-13. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00141

Schöberl, G. I., Kortekaas, K., Schöberl, F., & Kotrschal, K. (2014). Algorithm-supported visual error correction (AVEC) of heart rate measurements in dogs, Canis lupus familiaris. Behavior Research Methods.

Stephan, C., Steurer, M., & Aust, U. (2014). Discrimination of holograms and real objects by pigeons (Columba livia) and humans (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 128(3), 261-275. 10.1037/a0036874

Stöger-Horwath, A., Zeppelzauer, M., & Baotic, A. (2014). Age-group estimation in free-ranging African elephants based on acoustic cues of low-frequency rumbles. Bioacoustics : the international journal of animal sound and its recording, (DOI: 10.1080/09524622.2014.888375), 1-16. 10.1080/09524622.2014.888375

Stöger-Horwath, A., & Manger, P. (2014). Vocal learning in elephants: neural bases and adaptive context. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 28, 101-107. 10.1016/j.conb.2014.07.001

Szipl, G., Böckle, M., Werner, S. A. B., & Kotrschal, K. (2014). Mate Recognition and Expression of Affective State in Croop Calls of Northern Bald Ibis (Geronticus eremita). PLoS ONE, 9(2), e88265.

Szipl, G., & Bugnyar, T. (2014). Craving ravens: individual ‘haa’ call rates at feeding sites as cues to personality and levels of fission-fusion dynamics?. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 1(3), 265-280. 10.12966/abc.08.04.2014

Taylor, A., Cheke, L., Waismeyer, A., Meltzoff, A., Miller, R., Gopnik, A., ... Gray, R. (2014). Of babies and birds: complex tool behaviours are not sufficient for the evolution of the ability to create a causal intervention. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological sciences, 281(1787), 20140837.

Ujfalussy, D. J., Miklosi, A., Bugnyar, T., & Kotrschal, K. (2014). Role of Mental Representations in Quantity Judgments by Jackdaws (Corvus monedula). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 10.1037/a0034063

Viljoen, J. J., Ganswindt, A., Reynecke, C., Stöger-Horwath, A., & Langbauer Jr., W. R. (2014). Vocal stress associated with a translocation of a family herd of African elephants (Loxodonta africana) in the Kruger National Park, South Africa. Bioacoustics : the international journal of animal sound and its recording. DOI: 10.1080/09524622.2014.906320

Weisman, R., Höschele, M., & Sturdy, C. (2014). A comparative analysis of auditory perception in humans andsongbirds: A modular approach. Behavioural Processes, 104, 35-43.

Westphal Fitch, G., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2014). Towards a comparative approach to empirical aesthetics. in Art, Aesthetics, and the Brain. Oxford University Press.

Wilkins, A. S., Wrangham, R. W., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2014). The “Domestication Syndrome” in Mammals: A Unified Explanation Based on Neural Crest Cell Behavior and Genetics. Genetics: a periodical record of investigations bearing on heredity and variation, 197, 795-808. 10.1534/genetics.114.165423

Zeppelzauer, M., Hensman, S., & Stöger-Horwath, A. (2014). Towards an automated acoustic detection system for free-ranging elephants. Bioacoustics : the international journal of animal sound and its recording, (DOI: 10.1080/09524622.2014.906321).

2013

Auersperg, A., Szabo, B., von Bayern, A., & Bugnyar, T. (2013). Object Permanence in the Goffin Cockatoo (Cacatua goffini). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 10.1037/a0033272

Auersperg, A., Laumer, I. B., & Bugnyar, T. (2013). Goffin cockatoos wait for qualitative and quatitative gains but prefer ‘better’ to ‘more’. Biology Letters, 9(3). 10.1098/rsbl.2012.1092

Baotic, A., Stöger, A., Li, D., & Charlton, B. (2013). The vocal repertoire of infant giant pandas (Ailuropoda melanoleuca). Bioacoustics : the international journal of animal sound and its recording, 1-14.

Bowling, D., Herbst, C., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2013). Social origins of rhythm? Synchrony and temporal regularity in human vocalization. PLoS ONE, 8(11). doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0080402

Bowling, D. (2013). A vocal basis for the affective character of musical mode in melody. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(464).

Bugnyar, T. (2013). Social cognition in ravens. Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews .

Charlton, B., Frey, R., McKinnon, A. J., Fitch, W. T. S., & Reby, D. (2013). Koalas use a novel vocal organ to produce unusually low-pitched mating calls. Current Biology. 10.1016/j.cub.2013.10.069.

Di Lascio, F., Bshary, R., & Bugnyar, T. (2013). Ravens (Corvus corax) are indifferent to the gains of conspecific recipients or human partners in experimental tasks. Animal Cognition, 16(1), 35-43. 10.1007/s10071-012-0548-0

Filippi, P. (2013). Connessioni regolate: la chiave ontologica alle specie-specificità?. Epekeina. International Journal of Ontology, History and Critics, 2(1), 203-223. [DOI: 10.7408/epkn.epkn.v2i1.41].

Filippi, P. (2013). Specifically Human: Going Beyond Perceptual Syntax. Biosemiotics, 1-13. [10.1007/s12304-013-9187-3].

Fitch, W. T. S. (2013). Rhythmic cognition in humans and animals: distinguishing meter and pulse perception. Frontiers in Systems Neurosciences, 7(68). 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00068

Fitch, W. T. S., & Braccini, S. (2013). Primate laterality and the biology and evolution of human handedness: a review and synthesis. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 10.1111/nyas.12071

Fitch, W. T. S. (2013). Speech science: Tuned to the rhythm. Nature News & Views, 494. 10.1038/494434a

Garcia, M., Charlton, B. D., Wyman, M. T., Reby, D., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2013). Do Red Deer Stags (Cervus elaphus) Use Roar Fundamental Frequency (F0) to Assess Rivals?. PLoS ONE, 8(12). 10.1371/journal.pone.0083946

Gingras, B., Mohandesan, E., Boko, D., & Fitch, W. T. (2013). Phylogenetic signal in the acoustic parameters of the advertisement calls of four clades of anurans. BMC Evolutionary Biology, 13, 134. 10.1186/1471-2148-13-134

Gingras, B., & Fitch, W. T. (2013). A three-parameter model for classifying anurans into four genera based on advertisement calls. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America (JASA), 133(1), 547-59. 10.1121/1.4768878

Gingras, B., Marin, M. M., & Fitch, W. T. (2013). Beyond intensity: Spectral features effectively predict music-induced subjective arousal. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 10.1080/17470218.2013.863954

Gingras, B., Asselin, P-Y., & McAdams, S. (2013). Individuality in harpsichord performance: disentangling performer- and piece-specific influences on interpretive choices. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 895. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00895

Guillette, L., Höschele, M., Hahn, A., & Sturdy, C. (2013). Heterospecific discrimination of Poecile vocalization by zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata).Journal of Comparative Psychology, 127, 227.

Gunhold, T., Massen, J. J. M., & Bugnyar, T. (2013). Tradition Formation in Wild Common Marmosets. Folia Primatologica, 84(3-5).

Hahn, A., Guillette, L., Höschele, M., Mennill, D., Otter, K., Grava, T., Ratcliffe, L., & Sturdy, C. (2013). Dominance and geographic information contained within black-capped chickadee (Poecile atricapillus) song.Behaviour: an international journal of behaviourial biology, 1601.

Herbst, C., Svec, J., Frey, R., Gumpenberger, M., Stöger, A., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2013). Complex vibratory patterns in an elephant larynx. Journal of Experimental Biology, 4054-4064. 10.1242/jeb.091009

Herbst, C., Herzel, H., Svec, J., Wyman, M. T., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2013). Visualization of system dynamics using phasegrams. Journal of the Royal Society. Interface. 10.1098/rsif.2013.0288

Horn, L., Huber, L., & Range, F. (2013). The importance of the secure base effect for domestic dogs - evidence from a manipulative problem-solving task. PLoS ONE, 8(5), e65296. 10.1371/journal.pone.0065296

Horn, L., Range, F., & Huber, L. (2013). Dogs' attention towards humans depends on their relationship, not only on social familiarity. Animal Cognition, 16(3), 435-443. 10.1007/s10071-012-0584-9

Höschele, M., Weisman, R., Guillette, L., Hahn, A., & Sturdy, C. (2013). Chickadees fail standardized operant tests for octave equivalence. Animal Cognition, 16(4), 599. 10.1007/s10071-013-0597-z

Martins, C. I. M., Schaedelin, F. C., Mann, M., Blum, C., Mandl, I., Urban, D., & Grill, J. (2013). Exploring novelty: a component trait of behavioural syndromes in a colonial fish. Behaviour: an international journal of behaviourial biology, 149, 215-231.

Massen, J. J. M., & Sterck, E. H. M. (2013). Stability and Durability of Intra-and Intersex Social Bonds of Captive Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta). International Journal of Primatology, 34, 770–791.

Massen, J. J. M., Antonides, A., Arnold, A-M. K., Bionda, T., & Koski, S. E. (2013). A Behavioral View on Chimpanzee Personality: Exploration Tendency, Persistence, Boldness, and Tool‐Orientation Measured With Group Experiments. American Journal of Primatology, 75, 947–958.

Miller, R., & King, C. (2013). Husbandry training, using positive reinforcement techniques, for Marabou stork (Leptoptilos crumeniferus). International Zoo Yearbook, 47(47 DOI:10.1111/IZY.12001).

Pasukonis, A., Ringler, M., Brandl, H. B., Mangione, R., Ringler, E., & Hoedl, W. (2013). The Homing Frog: High Homing Performance in a Territorial Dendrobatid Frog Allobates femoralis (Dendrobatidae). Ethology, 119(9), 762-768. 10.1111/eth.12116

Preininger, D., Boeckle, M., Sztatecsny, M., & Hoedl, W. (2013). Divergent Receiver Responses to Components of Multimodal Signals in Two Foot-Flagging Frog Species. PLoS ONE, 8(1), [55367]. 10.1371/journal.pone.0055367

Preininger, D., Böckle, M., Freudmann, A., Starnberger, I., Sztatecsny, M., & Hödl, W. (2013). Multimodal signaling in the Small Torrent Frog (Micrixalus saxicola) in a complex acoustic environment.Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 67(9), 1449-1456. 10.1007/s00265-013-1489-6

Ravignani, A., Matellán Olivera, V., Gingras, B., Hofer, R., Rodríguez Hernández, C., Sonnweber, R-S., & Fitch, W. T. (2013). Primate drum kit: a system for studying acoustic pattern production by non-human primates using acceleration and strain sensors. Sensors, 13(8), 9790-820. 10.3390/s130809790

Ravignani, A., Sonnweber, R-S., Stobbe, N., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2013). Action at a distance: dependency sensitivity in a New World primate.Biology Letters.

Reber, S., Townsend , S., & Manser, M. (2013). Social monitoring via close calls in meerkats. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological sciences, 280(1765).

Riemer, S., Viranyi, Z., Müller, C., Huber, L., & Range, F. (2013). Choice of conflict resolution strategy is linked to sociability in dog puppies. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 36-44.

Ringler, E., Pasukonis, A., Hoedl, W., & Ringler, M. (2013). Characterization of seven new polymorphic microsatellite loci in the brilliant-thighed poison frog Allobates femoralis (Dendrobatidae), and their cross-species utility in three other dendrobatid species. Herpetological Journal, 23(3), 175-178.

Ringler, E., Pasukonis, A., Hödl, W., & Ringler, M. (2013). Tadpole transport logistics in a Neotropical poison frog: indications for strategic planning and adaptive plasticity in anuran parental care. Frontiers in Zoology, 10(1), 67. 10.1186/1742-9994-10-67

Sonnweber, R-S., Stobbe, N., Romero, O. Z., Slice, D. E., Fieder, M., Wallner, B., & Sonnweber, R-S. (2013). A New Method for the Analysis of Soft Tissues with Data Acquired under Field Conditions. PLoS ONE, 8(6), [67521]. 10.1371/journal.pone.0067521

Stephan, C., & Bugnyar, T. (2013). Pigeons integrate past knowledge across sensory modalities. Animal Behaviour, 85, 605-613.

Taylor, A., Miller, R., & Gray, R. (2013). Clear evidence of habituation counters counterbalancing. Response to Dymond, S., Haselgrove, M. and McGregor, A. Clever crows or unbalanced birds? A commentary on Taylor et al. (2012). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America (PNAS).

Taylor, A., Miller, R., & Gray, R. (2013). Reply to Boogert et al.: The devil is unlikely to be in association or distraction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America (PNAS).

Teschke, I., Wascher, C. A. F., Scriba, M. F., von Bayern, A. M. P., Huml, V., Siemers, B., & Tebbich, S. (2013). Did tool-use evolve with enhanced physical cognitive abilities?. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Biological Sciences, 368(1630). 10.1098/rstb.2012.0418

Ujfalussy, D. J., Miklosi, A., Bugnyar, T., & Kotrschal, K. (2013). Role of Mental Representations in Quantity Judgments by Jackdaws (Corvus monedula). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 10.1037/a0034063

Wallis, L., Range, F., Müller, C., Serisier, S., Huber, L., & Viranyi, Z. (2013). Age effects on interspecific communicative abilities of domestic dogs. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research, 8(4).

Wascher, C. A. F., & Bugnyar, T. (2013). Behavioral Responses to Inequity in Reward Distribution and Working Effort in Crows and Ravens. PLoS ONE, 8(2). 10.1371/journal.pone.0056885

Wascher, C., & Bugnyar, T. (2013). Awareness to inequity and working effort in carrion crows (Corvus corone corone) and common ravens (Corvus corax). PLoS ONE, 8(2). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0056885

Westphal Fitch, G., Oh, J., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2013). Studying aesthetics with the method of production: effects of context and local symmetry. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7(1), 13-26. 10.1037/a0031795

Westphal Fitch, G., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2013). Spatial analysis of "crazy quilts'', a class of potentially random aesthetic artefact. PLoS ONE, 8(9), e74055. 10.1371/journal.pone.0074055

2012

Albiach-Serrano, A., Bugnyar, & T., Call, J. (2012). Apes (Gorilla gorilla, Pan paniscus, P. troglodytes, Pongo pygmaeus) vs. corvids (Corvus corax, C. corone) in a support task: the effect of pattern and functionality. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(4), 355-367.

Aust, U. (2012). The Vienna comparative cognition technology (VCCT): An innovative operant conditioning system for various species and experimental procedures.Behavior Research Methods, 44(4), 909-918.

Aust, U. (2012). Learning of an oddity rule by pigeons in a four-choice touch-screen procedure.Animal Cognition. 10.1007/s10071-012-0574

Braccini, S., Lambeth, S., Schapiro, S., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2012). Eye preferences in captive chimpanzees. Animal Cognition, 15(5), 971-978. 10.1007/s10071-012-0523-9

Braun, A., Walsdorff, T., Fraser, O., & Bugnyar, T. (2012). Socialized sub-groups in a temporary stable raven flock?. Journal of Ornithology, 153(1), 97-104. 10.1007/s10336-011-0810-2

Braun, A., & Bugnyar, T. (2012). Social bonds and rank acquisition in raven nonbreeder aggregations. Animal Behaviour, 84(6), 1507-1515.

 

Böckle, M., & Bugnyar, T. (2012). Long-Term Memory for Affiliates in Ravens. Current Biology, 22(9), 801-806.

Böckle, M., Szipl, G., & Bugnyar, T. (2012). Who wants food? Individual characteristics in raven yells. Animal Behaviour, 84(5), 1123-1130.

Charlton, B., Filippi, P., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2012). Do women prefer more complex music around ovulation?. PLoS ONE, 7(4). 10.1371/journal.pone.0035626

Charlton, B. D., Reby, D., Ellis, W. A. H., Brumm, J., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2012). Estimating the Active Space of Male Koala Bellows: Propagation of Cues to Size and Identity in a Eucalyptus Forest. PLoS ONE, 7(9). 10.1371/journal.pone.0045420

Charlton, B. D., Ellis, W. A. H., Brumm, J., Nilsson, K., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2012). Female koalas prefer bellows in which lower formants indicate larger males. Animal Behaviour, 84(6), 1565-1571. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.09.034

Dufour, V., Wascher, C., Braun, A., Miller, R., & Bugnyar, T. (2012). Corvids can decide if a future exchange is worth waiting for. Biology Letters, 8(2), 201-204. 

Fitch, W. T. S., Hagoort, P., & Friederici, A. D. (2012). Pattern perception and computational complexity: introduction to the special issue. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Biological Sciences, 367(1598), 1925-1932.

Fitch, W. T. S., & Friederici, A. D. (2012). Artificial grammar learning meets formal language theory: an overview. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Biological Sciences, 367(1598), 1933-1955. 10.1098/rstb.2012.0103.

Fitch, W. T. S., Charlton, B. D., Larkin, R., & Ellis, W. A. H. (2012). Perception of size-related formant information in male koalas (Phascolarctos cinereus). Animal Cognition, 15(5), 999-1006.

Fitch, W. T. S. (2012). Evolutionary Developmental Biology and Human Language Evolution: Constraints on Adaptation. Evolutionary Biology, 39(4, SI), 613-637. 10.1007/s11692-012-9162-y

Ghazanfar, A. A., Takahashi, D. Y., Mathur, N., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2012). Cineradiography of Monkey Lip-Smacking Reveals Putative Precursors of Speech Dynamics. Current Biology, 22(13), 1176-1182. 10.1016/j.cub.2012.04.055

Gingras, B., Böckle, M., Herbst, C., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2012). Call acoustics reflect body size across four genera of anurans. Journal of Zoology, 289(2), 143-150. 10.1111/j.1469-7998.2012.00973.x

Herbst, C., Duus, E., Jers, H., & Svec, J. (2012). Quantitative Voice Class Assessment of Amateur Choir Singers: A Pilot Investigation. International Journal of Research in Choral Singing (IJRCS), 4(1), 47-59.

Herbst, C. (2012). Freddie Mercury - Akustische Stimm-Analyse. L.O.G.O.S. Interdisziplinaer, 20(3), 174-183.

Herbst, C., Stöger, A., Frey, R., Lohscheller, J., Titze, I. R., Gumpenberger, M., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2012). How low can you go? Physical production mechanism of elephant infrasonic vocalization. Science, 337, 595-599.

Herbst, C. (2012). An Elephant's Silent Call. Science.

Herler, A., & Stöger, A. (2012). Vocalizations and associated behaviour of Asian elephant (Elephas maximus) calves. Behaviour: an international journal of behaviourial biology, 149(6), 575-599. 10.1163/156853912X648516

Horn, L., Viranyi, Z., Miklósi, Á., Huber, L., & Range, F. (2012). Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) flexibly adjust their human-directed behavior to the actions of their human partners in a problem situation. Animal Cognition, 15, 57-71.

Huber, L., & Wilkinson, A. (2012). Radial-arm maze navigation of the red-footed tortoises (Geochelone carbonaria). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(3), 305-317.

Huber, L., Fitch, W. T. S., Westphal Fitch, G., & Gómez, J. C. (2012). Production and perception rules underlying visual patterns: effects of symmetry and hierarchy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Biological Sciences, 367, 2007-2022.

Loretto, M-C., Stöwe, M., & Bugnyar, T. (2012). Does coping style affect preferences for feeding sites in ravens?. Wiener Tierärztliche Monatsschrift, 99(1), 62-63.

Loretto, M-C., Fraser, O., & Bugnyar, T. (2012). Ontogeny of dominance relations and coalitions in common ravens (Corvus corax).International Journal of Comparative Psychology (IJCP).

Massen, J. J. M., Vermunt, D. A., & Sterck, E. H. M. (2012). Male yawning is more contagious than female yawning among chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).PLoS ONE, 7(7). 10.1371/journal.pone.0040697

Massen, J. J. M. (2012). Sneaky monkeys: an audience effect on rhesus macaque (Macaca mulatta) sexual behaviour.American Journal of Primatology, 74, 217-228. 10.1002/ajp.21988

Massen, J. J. M. (2012). Chimps catch yawns from dominant males. New Scientist: the global science and technology weekly, (2875), 15-15.

Massen, J. J. M. (2012). Inequity aversion and the effect of relationship quality in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). American Journal of Primatology, 74, 145-156. 10.1002/ajp.21014

Müller, C., Riemer, S., Range, F., & Huber, L. (2012). Brief owner absence does not induce negative judgement bias in pet dogs. Animal Cognition, 15(5), 1031-1035.

Schlögl, H. C., Schmidt, J., Böckle, M., Weiß, B. M., & Kotrschal, K. (2012). Grey parrots use inferential reasoning based on acoustic cues alone. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological sciences, 22(279), 4142-4153. 10.1098/rspb.2012.1292

Schrauf, C., Call, J., Fuwa, K., & Hirata, S. (2012). Do Chimpanzees Use Weight to Select Hammer Tools?. PLoS ONE, 7(7). 10.1371/journal.pone.0041044

Schwab, C., Swoboda, R., Kotrschal, K., & Bugnyar, T. (2012). Recipients Affect Prosocial and Altruistic Choices in Jackdaws, Corvus monedula. PLoS ONE, 7(4). 10.1371/journal.pone.0034922

Steurer, M., Stephan, C., Aringer, J., Berger, W., & Aust, U. (2012). The Multi-Stimulus box: An innovative learning device for the comparative study on object perception and recognition with various types of stimuli. Behavior Research Methods, 44(3), 725-732. 10.3758/s13428-011-0179-4

Stobbe, N., Westphal Fitch, G., Aust, U., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2012). Visual artificial grammar learning: comparative research on humans, kea (Nestor notabilis) and pigeons (Columba livia). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Biological Sciences, 367(1598, SI), 1995-2006. 10.1098/rstb.2012.0096

Stöger, A., Mietchen, D., Oh, S., de Silva, S., Herbst, C., Kwon, S., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2012). An Asian elephant imitates human speech. Current Biology, 22, 1-5.

Stöger, A., Heilmann, G., Zeppelzauer, M., Ganswindt, A., Hensman, S., & Charlton, B. D. (2012). Visualizing sound emission of elephant vocalizations: Evidence for two rumble production types. PLoS ONE, 7(11). 10.1371/journal.pone.0048907

Stöger, A., Baotic, A., Li, D., & Charlton, B. D. (2012). Acoustic Features Indicate Arousal in Infant Giant Panda Vocalisations. Ethology, 118(9), 896-905.

Sztatecsny, M., Preininger, D., Loretto, M-C., & Hödl, W. (2012). Don't get the blues: conspicuous nuptial colouration of male moor frogs (Rana arvalis) supports visual mate recognition during scramble competition in large breeding aggregations. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 66(12), 1587-1593. 10.1007/s00265-012-1412-6

Taylor, A. H., Miller, R., & Gray, R. D. (2012). New Caledonian crows reason about hidden causal agents. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America (PNAS), 109(40), 16389-16391.

Tebbich, S., Stankewitz, S., & Teschke, I. (2012). Investigating the relationship between feeding ecology, flexibility and neophobia in two species of Darwin’s finches. Ethology, (118), 135-146. 

Tebbich, S., Teschke, I., Cartmill, E. A., & Stankewitz, S. (2012). Use of a barbed tool by an adult and a juvenile woodpecker finch (Cactospiza pallida). Behavioural Processes, 89(2), 166-171.

Tebbich, S., Stankewitz, S., & Teschke, I. (2012). The Relationship Between Foraging, Learning Abilities and Neophobia in Two Species of Darwin’s Finches. Ethology, 118(2), 135-146. 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.02001.x

Tönshoff, E., Penz, T., Narzt, T., Collingro, A., Schmitz-Esser, S., Pfeiffer, S., Klepal, W., Wagner, M., Weinmaier, T., Rattei, T., & Horn, M. (2012). Bacteriocyte-associated gammaproteobacterial symbionts of the Adelges nordmannianae/piceae complex (Hemiptera: Adelgidae): Adelgidae). The ISME Journal: multidisciplinary journal of microbial ecology, 6(2), 384-396. 10.1038/ismej.2011.102

Ujfalussy, D. J., Bugnyar, T., & Miklosi, A. (2012). Ontogeny of object permanence in a non-storing corvid species, the jackdaw (Corvus monedula). Animal Cognition.

Wascher, C., Dufour, V., & Bugnyar, T. (2012). Carrion crows cannot overcome impulsive choice in a quantitative exchange task. Frontiers in Comparative Psychology, (3:118). 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00118

Wascher, C., Szipl, G., Böckle, M., & Wilkinson, A. C. (2012). You sound familiar: carrion crows can differentiate between the calls of known and unknown heterospecifics. Animal Cognition, 15(5), 1015-1019. 10.1007/s10071-012-0508-8

Book chapters

Boeckle, M. (2012). The Notion of Kowledge in Cognitive Ethology: Cognitive Ethology in the Realm of the Red Queen (pp. 1–125). Saarbrücken, Germany: AV Akademikerverlag.

Bugnyar, T., Boyd, R., Bossan, B., Gächter, S., Griffiths, T., Hammerstein, P., Jensen, K., Mussweiler, T., Nagel, R. & Warneken, F. (2012) Evolutionary perspectives on social cognition. In: Evolution and the Mechanisms of Decision Making (ed. Hammerstein, P. & Stevens, J.), Ernst Strüngmann Forum Reports. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 345-367.

Fitch WT, Westphal-Fitch G. General Introduction. In: Fitch WT, Westphal-Fitch G, Editors. Language Evolution. Abingdon: Routledge. 2012. p. 1-19

Fitch, W. T. (2012). "The Biology and Evolution of Rhythm: Unravelling a Paradox," in Language and Music as Cognitive Systems, edited by P. Rebuschat, M. Rohmeier, J. A. Hawkins, and I. Cross (Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK), pp. 73-95.

Fitch, W. T. (2012). "Reweaving the strands: Welcoming diverse perspectives on the biology of music," in Language and Music as Cognitive Systems, edited by P. Rebuschat, M. Rohmeier, J. A. Hawkins, and I. Cross (Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK), pp. 128-136.

Fitch, W. T. (2012). "The Evolution of Language," in Communication: From its origin to internet, edited by M. d. Moragas (Catalunya Literaria Fundacio Privada, Tarragona, Spain), pp. 27-47.

Fitch, W. T. (2012). "La evolución biológica del lenguaje," in La communicacion: De los orignenes a Internet, edited by M. d. Moragas (Catalunya Literaria Fundacio Privada, Barcelona, Spain), pp. 27-48.

O´Hara M (2012) Solve Difficult Problems and Outsmart Researchers. In: Watanabe S. (ed.) Logic and Sensibility. Keio University Press.

Wilkinson A., Huber L. (2012) Cold-Blooded Cognition: Reptilian Cognitive Abilities in The Oxford Handbook of Comparative Evolutionary Psychology (ed. Jennifer Vonk,Todd K. Shackelford) Oxford University Press

2011

Amann, L., Huber, L., & Gajdon, G. (2011). Keas rely on social information in a tool use task but abandon it in favour of overt exploration. Interaction Studies, 12(2), 304-323. 10.1075/is.12.2.06gaj

Auer, M., Wedl, M., Range, F., Viranyi, Z., Belenyi, B., & Kotrschal, K. (2011). Leash walking as a model for cooperation between humans and wolves: the effects of personality and intensity of contact. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research, 6(1), 100-101. 

Bugnyar, T. (2011). Knower-guesser differentiation in ravens: others' viewpoints matter.Proceedings of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological sciences.

Bugnyar, T., & Stadler, F. (2011). Persönliche Paradigmen - Der Rabenflüsterer. Info Das Magazin des Wissenschaftsfonds, (78), 32-35.

Bugnyar, T., Kenward, B., Schlögl, C., Kacelnik, A., Rutz, C., & Weir, A. A. S. (2011). The evolutionary origin and ontogeny of tool use in New Caledonian crows: a new hypothesis. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society: a journal of evolution. 

Bugnyar, T., & Pika, S. (2011). The use of referential gestures in ravens (Corvus corax) in the wild. Nature Communications, 2.

Charlton, B., & Reby, D. (2011). Context-related acoustic variation in male fallow deer (Dama dama) groans. PLoS ONE. 10.1371/journal.pone.0021066

Charlton, B., Swaisgood, R. R., Keating, J. L., Huang, Y., Rengui, L., & Kersey, D. (2011). Vocal cues to male androgen levels in giant pandas. Biology Letters, 7(1), 71-74.

Charlton, B., & Reby, D. (2011). Attention grabbing in red deer sexual calls. Animal Cognition.

Charlton, B., Wyman, M. T., Reby, D., & Locatelli, Y. (2011). Variability of female responses to conspecific vs. heterospecific male mating calls in polygynous deer: an open door to hybridization?. PLoS ONE. 10.1371/journal.pone.0023296

Charlton, B., Fitch, W. T. S., Ellis, W. A. H., Nilsson, K., Brumm, J., & McKinnon, A. J. (2011). Perception of Male Caller Identity in Koalas (Phascolarctos cinereus): Acoustic Analysis and Playback Experiments. PLoS ONE, 6(5). 10.1371/journal.pone.0020329

Dufour, V., Wascher, C., Braun, A., Miller, R., & Bugnyar, T. (2011). Time is money: Corvids can decide if a future exchange is worth waiting for. Biology Letters.

Dufour, V., Wascher, C., Braun, A., Miller, R., & Bugnyar, T. (2011). Time is money: Corvids can decide if a future transaction is worth waiting for. Biology Letters, 23(8), 201-204.

Fessl, B., Loaiza, A. D., Tebbich, S., & Young, G. H. (2011). Feeding and nesting requirements of the critically endangered Mangrove Finch (Camarhynchus heliobates). Journal of Ornithology, 152(2), 453-460. 10.1007/s10336-010-0610-0

Fitch, W. T. S., Charlton, B., Ellis, W. A. H., McKinnon, A. J., Nilsson, K., Brumm, J., & Cowin, G. J. (2011). Cues to body size in the formant spacing of male koala (Phascolarctos cinereus) bellows: honesty in an exaggerated trait. Journal of Experimental Biology, (214), 3414-3422. 10.1242/?jeb.061358

Fitch, W. T. S. (2011). Genes, Language, Cognition, and Culture: Towards Productive Inquiry. Human Biology (Detroit): the international journal of population biology and genetics, 83(2), 323-329.

Fitch, W. T. S. (2011). Biological versus cultural evolution: Beyond a false dichotomy: Comment on “Modeling the cultural evolution of language” by Luc Steels. Physics of Life Reviews, 8(4), 357-358.

Fitch, W. T. S. (2011). Unity and diversity in human language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Biological Sciences, 366(1563), 376-388.

Fitch, W. T. S. (2011). The evolution of syntax: An exaptationist perspective. Frontiers in Evolutionary Neuroscience, 3(9), 1-12. 10.3389/fnevo.2011.00009

Fitch, W. T. S. (2011). Speech perception: a language-trained chimpanzee weighs in.Current Biology, 21(14), 543-546.

Fraser, O., & Bugnyar, T. (2011). Ravens reconcile after conflicts with valuable partners. PLoS ONE. 10.1371/journal.pone.0018118

Gajdon, G., & Huber, L. (2011). Navigating a tool end in a specific direction: stick-tool use in kea ( Nestor notabilis). Biology Letters.

Gajdon, G., Huber, L., & Liedtke, J. (2011). Big brains are not enough: performance of three parrot species in the trap-tube paradigm. Animal Cognition, 14(1), 143-149.

Gingras, B., Lagrandeur-Ponce, T., McAdams, S., & Giordano, B. L. (2011). Perceiving musical identity: Performer identification is dependent on performer expertise and on expressiveness, but not on listener expertise. Perception, 40(10), 1206-1220.

Grant, V. J., Konecna, M., Sonnweber, R-S., Irwin, R. J., & Wallner, B. (2011). Macaque mothers' preconception testosterone levels relate to dominance and to sex of offspring. Animal Behaviour, 82(4), 893-899. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.07.029

Herbst, C., Schutte, H. K., & Qiu, Q. (2011). Membranous and cartilaginous vocal fold adduction in singing. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America (JASA), 129(4), 2253-2262.

Huber, L., Gajdon, G., Kacelnik, A., & von Bayern, A. (2011). Flexibility in problem solving and tool use of kea and New Caledonian crows in a Multi Access Box paradigm. PLoS ONE, 6. 10.1371/journal.pone.0020231

Huber, L., & Wilkinson, A. (2011). No evidence in contagious yawning in the red-footed tortoise (Geochelone carbonaria). Current Zoology, 57(4), 477-484.

Huber, L., Range, F., & Müller, C. (2011). Female but not male dogs respond to a size constancy violation. Biology Letters.

Huber, L., Range, F., & Horn, L. (2011). Dogs' (Canis familiaris) attention toward humans: does it depend on the relationship?. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research, 6(1), 76-76.

Huber, L., Range, F., & Heyes, C. (2011). Automatic imitation in dogs. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological sciences, 278, 211-217.

Huber, L., Gajdon, G., Fujita, K., & Miyata, H. (2011). How do keas (Nestor notabilis) solve artificial-fruit problems with multiple locks?. Animal Cognition, 14(1), 45-58.

Kenward, B., Schlögl, H. C., Rutz, C., Weir, A. A. S., Bugnyar, T., & Kacelnik, A. (2011). On the evolutionary and ontogenetic origins of tool-oriented behaviour in New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides). Biological Journal of the Linnean Society: a journal of evolution, 102(4), 870-877. 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2011.01613.x

Müller, J. (2011). Can your reptiles recognise you?. Practical Reptile Keeping.

Müller, J., & Wilkinson, A. (2011). Reptilian cognition – teaching tortoises. Practical Reptile Keeping. 

Müller, J. (2011). How reptiles find their way. Practical Reptile Keeping.

Range, F., Hentrup, M., & Viranyi, Z. (2011). Dogs are able to solve a means-end tasks. Animal Cognition, 14(4), 575-583. 

Range, F., & Viranyi, Z. (2011). Commentary: Evaluating the logic of perspective taking experiments. Learning and Behavior.

Range, F., Huber, L., Viranyi, Z., Horn, L., & Miklosi, A. (2011). Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) flexibly adjust their human- directed behavior to the actions of their human partners in a problem situation. Animal Cognition, 15(1), 57-71.

Schlögl, C., Schmidt, J., Scheid, C., Kotrschal, K., & Bugnyar, T. (2011). Gaze direction - a cue for hidden food in rooks (Corvus frugilegus)?. Behavioural Processes, 88(2), 88-93. 10.1016/j.beproc.2011.08.002

Schrauf, C., & Call, J. (2011). Great Apes Use Weight as a Cue to Find Hidden Food. American Journal of Primatology, 73(4), 323-334. 10.1002/ajp.20899

Stöger, A., Charlton, B., Kratochvil, H., & Fitch, W. T. S. (2011). Vocal cues indicate level of arousal in infant African elephant roars. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America (JASA), 130(3), 1700-1710. 

Tebbich, S., Stankewitz, S., & Teschke, I. (2011). 2011 Investigating the relationship between feeding ecology, flexibility and neophobia in two species of Darwin’s finches. Ethology, 117, 1-12.

Tebbich, S., Dvorak, M., Kleindorfer, S., Nemeth, E., & Fessl, B. (2011). Distribution and abundance of Darwin’s finches and other land birds on Santa Cruz Island, Galápagos: evidence for declining populations. Oryx.

Teschke, I., Cartmill, E. A., & Stankewitz, S. (2011). Sometimes Tool-use is Not the Key: No Evidence for Cognitive Adaptive Specializations in Tool-using Woodpecker Finches. Animal Behaviour, (82), 945-956.

Teschke, I., Cartmill, E. A., Stankewitz, S., & Tebbich, S. (2011). Sometimes tool use is not the key: No evidence for cognitive adaptive specializations in tool-using woodpecker finches. Animal Behaviour, 82(5), 945-956. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.07.032

Teschke, I., & Tebbich, S. (2011). Physical cognition and tool-use: performance of Darwin's finches in the two-trap tube task. Animal Cognition, 14(4), 555-563. 10.1007/s10071-011-0390-9

Viranyi, Z., & Range, F. (2011). Development of Gaze following abilities in wolves (Canis lupus). PLoS ONE, 6(2). 10.1371/journal.pone.0016888

 

Book chapters

Fitch, W. T. (2011). "Deep Homology" in the Biology & Evolution of Language. In A. M. Di Sciullo & C. Boeckx (Eds.), The Biolinguistic Enterprise: New Perspectives on the Evolution and Nature of the Human Language Faculty. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 135-166

Huber, L. (in press). What, whom and how: Selectivity in social learning. In P. F. Ferrari & F. de Waal (Eds.), The Primate Mind. Harvard University Press.

Huber, L. (in press). Social Learning in Animals. In N. Seel (Ed.), Encyclopedia of the Sciences of Learning. Wien, New York: Springer.

Huber, L. (in press). Zur Evolution von Erkenntnis und Moral aus der Sicht der Kognitionsbiologie. In H. P. Weber & R. Langthaler (Eds.), Evolution und Schöpfung. Wien: Vienna University Press.

Huber, L., & Aust, U. (in press). A modified feature theory as an account of pigeon visual categorization. In E. Wasserman & T. Zentall (Eds.), Handbook of Comparative Cognition. New York: Oxdord University Press.

Huber, L., & Wilkinson, A. (in press). Cognitive Evolution: A Comparative Approach. In F. G. Barth, P. Giampieri-Deutsch & H.-D. Klein (Eds.), Sensory Perception: Mind and Matter. Wien, New York: Springer.

2010

Auersperg, A. M. I., Gajdon, G. K., & Huber, L. (2010). Kea, Nestor notabilis, produce dynamic relationships between objects in a second-order tool use task. , 80(5), 783-789.

Aust, U., & Huber, L. (2010). Representational insight in pigeons: Comparing subjects with and without real-life experience. , 13, 207–218.

Aust, U., & Huber, L. (2010). The role of skin-related information in pigeons' categorization and recognition of humans in pictures. , 50, 1941-1948.

Braccini, S., Lambeth, S., Schapiro, S., & Fitch, W. T. (2010). Bipedal tool use strengthens chimpanzee hand preferences. , 58(3), 234-241.

Faragó, T., Pongracz, P., Miklósi, Á., Huber, L., Virányi, Z., & Range, F. (2010). Dogs' expectation about signalers' body size by virtue of their growls. , 5(12), e15175.

 

Fessl, B., Young G. H., Young R. P., Rodríguez-Matamoros, J., Dvorak, M., Tebbich, S., Fa, J. E: 2010 How to save the rarest Darwin's finch from extinction: the mangrove finch on Isabela Island. April 12, 365:1019-1030; doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0288 [IF 5.117, 2 citations]

Fessl, B , Loaiza, A.D.,Tebbich, S., Young G.H. 2010 Feeding and nesting requirements of the critically endangered Mangrove Finch . online. Doi: 10.1007/s10336-010-0610-0 [IF 1.476,]

 

Fitch, W. T., Huber, L., & Bugnyar, T. (2010). Social cognition and the evolution of language: constructing cognitive phylogenies. , 65(6), 795-814.

Fraser, O. N., & Bugnyar, T. (2010). Do ravens show consolation? Responses to distressed others. , 5(5), e10605.

Herbst, C. T., Fitch, W. T., & Svec, J. G. (2010). Electroglottographic wavegrams: a technique for visualizing vocal fold dynamics noninvasively. , 128(5), 3070-3078.

Schiel, N., Souto, A., Huber, L., & Bezerra, B. M. (2010). Hunting strategies in wild common marmosets are prey and age dependent. , 71, 1–8.

Tebbich, S., Sterelny, K., Teschke, I. 2010 The tale of the finch: adaptive radiation and behavioural flexibility. April 12,365:1099-1109; doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0291[IF 5.117,3 citations]

 Titze, I. R., Fitch, W. T., Hunter, E. J., Alipour, F., Montequin, D., Armstrong, D. L., et al. (2010). Vocal power and pressure-flow relationships in excised tiger larynges, 213(Pt 22), 3866-3873.

Wascher, C. A. F., Fraser, O. N., & Kotrschal, K. (2010). Heart Rate during Conflicts Predicts Post-Conflict Stress-Related Behavior in Greylag Geese. , 5(12), e15751.

Wilkinson, A., Kuenstner, K., Mueller, J., & Huber, L. (2010). Social learning in a non-social reptile (Geochelone carbonaria). , 6(5), 614-616.

Wilkinson, A., Mandl, I., Bugnyar, T., & Huber, L. (2010). Gaze following in the red-footed tortoise (Geochelone carbonaria). , 13, 765–769.

Wilkinson, A., Specht, H. L., & Huber, L. (2010). Pigeons can discriminate group mates from strangers using the concept of familiarity. , 80, 109–115.

 Books and Book chapters

 Fitch, W. T. (2010): Evolution of Language, Cambridge University Press, New York, 2010

Huber, L. (2010). Ein vorsprachliches ICH? Evolutionsbiologische Aspekte. In H. Düringer, H. Meisinger & W.-R. Schmidt (Eds.), (pp. 51–75). Hanau: Haag und Herchen Verlag.

Huber, L. (2010). Kognition und Moral bei menschlichen und nicht-menschlichen Tieren: homologe und analoge Gemeinsamkeiten. , 6, 41–43.

Huber, L. (2010). Categories and Concepts: Language-Related Competences in Non-Linguistic Species. In M. D. Breed & J. Moore (Eds.), (pp. 261–266). Oxford: Academic Press.